Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View


Free download. Book file PDF easily for everyone and every device. You can download and read online Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View file PDF Book only if you are registered here. And also you can download or read online all Book PDF file that related with Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View book. Happy reading Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Bookeveryone. Download file Free Book PDF Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View at Complete PDF Library. This Book have some digital formats such us :paperbook, ebook, kindle, epub, fb2 and another formats. Here is The CompletePDF Book Library. It's free to register here to get Book file PDF Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Pocket Guide.
Alzheimer’s disease redefined as a ‘major killer’

Age-related grey matter changes can be reflected by cortical thinning, a decreased brain tissue surface, or a progressive reduction of brain volume after the second decade of life [ 22—24 ]. Reasons for this decline are a shrinkage of neurons, a reduction of synaptic spines, and a reduced number of synapses rather than neuronal loss [ 6 ]. Global grey matter volume loss seems to show a relatively constant progression until the age of 70 [ 23 ] with an annual atrophy rate of approximately 0. Afterwards, however, longitudinal data suggest an accelerated degeneration 0.

Noteworthy, more recent longitudinal studies reported decelerated changes in the prefrontal cortex and other brain regions as well, indicating that the pattern and rates of grey matter atrophy in older adults are highly region-dependent [ 24 ]. Moreover, grey matter volume reductions do not equally involve all brain regions.

Whereas reductions are consistently reported for superior parietal and inferior temporal brain regions as well as for hippocampus, insula, caudate, putamen, and particularly prefrontal cortex [ 6, 26, 28—32 ], other brain regions as parahippocampal gyrus, cingulate cortex, or occipital brain regions appear to be relatively spared from age-related change [ 30 ].

In fact, there is evidence for a specific vulnerability of frontostriatal brain regions: Bauer and colleagues, for example, used voxel-based morphometry to identify pronounced age-related grey matter volume reductions in the insula, the nucleus caudatus, and the prefrontal cortex, whereas grey matter atrophy was not evident in other brain regions [ 4 ]. To be able to draw conclusions on the meaning of these grey matter changes for cognition, most brain imaging studies related regional brain volume to cognitive performance in specific cognitive subdomains. Longitudinal data revealed an association between frontal brain volume in seniors and decreased executive task performance 12 months later [ 33 ].

Other studies also consistently reported correlations between frontal brain volume and memory, attention, and executive task performance [ 4, 11, 34, 35 ]. These results highlight the relevance of prefrontal brain structures for different cognitive processes, which is confirmed by functional imaging data [ 36 ]. Since working memory can be comprehended as an interface between memory, attention, and executive processes, it is not surprising that numerous studies found relations between frontal brain volume and working memory performance as well e.

Finally, striatal and hippocampal grey matter volume appear to be important predictors for memory performance in older adults, although an association between hippocampal volume and memory was not observed in all studies [ 4, 34 ]. Moreover, many of the findings do not necessarily imply that hippocampal grey matter volume reduces with age, because the same relationship may be found in younger individuals. In fact, findings on age-related hippocampal shrinkage are partly heterogeneous as well: Although many studies revealed a negative relationship between hippocampal grey matter volume and age e.

Taken together, decreased cognitive performances in working memory, attention, and executive functions can at least partly be attributed to age-related grey matter atrophy of prefrontal brain regions. Age-related white matter changes are particularly characterized by the deterioration of fiber connections caused by the loss of myelin. As a consequence, the length of myelinated axons and white matter volume are reduced in older adults [ 6 ]. Contrary to grey matter volume loss, however, white matter volume reduction shows a different pattern. The results of Good and colleagues, for example, did not reveal a reduction of global white matter volume across the lifespan at all [ 23 ].

However, results suggested an inverted U-shaped pattern in female subjects which was confirmed by more recent research. Westlye and colleagues examined individuals between 8 and 85 years of age and showed that global white matter volume increased until an age 50 years to decrease afterwards [ 39—41 ]. This inverted U-shaped pattern of white matter volume loss was observed for multiple brain regions. Noteworthy, the development of neuroimaging techniques in the last decade has yielded advanced methods to examine white matter changes in older individuals. Diffusion tensor imaging DTI , for example, provides the opportunity to examine microstructural white matter alterations with advancing age.

DTI provides different measures for the structural integrity of fiber systems, the most prominent of which are fractional anisotropy FA and mean diffusivity MD. FA and MD indicate the integrity of axons and cellular membranes. Thereby, higher FA values and lower MD values reflect a higher degree of structural integrity.

Similar to age-related white volume loss, changes in FA seem to follow an inverted U-shaped pattern with a maximum at an age of thirty years followed by a continuous but slight decrease until an age of 65 years and an accelerating decrease afterwards [ 41, 42 ]. Changes in MD show an analog pattern U-shaped providing further evidence for a three-stage model of white matter alterations across the lifespan with accelerated changes in early and late life decades.

Similar to some results on white matter volume, regional differences seem to be rather discreet. In fact, changes of white matter integrity were not only observed in frontal white matter but also in other brain regions [ 41 ], indicating that age-related alterations in FA and MD follow a similar course in all brain regions. Thereby, most findings revealed stronger associations between FA and cognitive performance than between MD and cognitive performance, indicating that age-related FA reductions seem to be more relevant for cognitive changes than increases in MD [ 43 ].

However, similar to the accentuated frontal grey matter atrophy across the lifespan, other studies point toward a specific vulnerability of frontal fiber connections [ 5, 23, 44, 45 ]. In fact, many studies reported an anterior-posterior gradient of FA reductions in older individuals indicating a specific vulnerability of prefrontal connections with advancing age [ 9, 46—50 ]. A similar anterior-posterior gradient was reported for the increase in MD [ 9, 46 ]. However, the anterior-posterior gradient of white matter degeneration does not seem to be universally valid [ 51 ] but may particularly apply to specific fiber systems such as afferent and efferent prefrontal connections [ 52 ].

Salat concluded that small and less myelinated fibers are particularly vulnerable to age-related decline, such as fiber tracts whose myelinization is completed in later life. To examine the impact of these age-related white matter changes on cognitive performance, regional white matter volume and integrity were correlated with performance in different cognitive domains.

Most studies reported positive relations of frontal white matter volume and integrity with processing speed, executive functioning, and working memory [ 8—10, 44 ]. Noteworthy, most of these cognitive processes do not only rely on the integrity of frontal white matter but also on connections between frontal and other task-related brain regions. Sasson and colleagues, for example, reported an association between the integrity of anterior-posterior connections and executive functioning [ 53 ].

Taken together, there is much empirical evidence for increasingly defective anterior and posterior fiber tracks with advancing age. These deficiencies lead to a cortical disconnection impeding the communication between the prefrontal cortex and other brain regions and leading to decreased cognitive performance particularly in executive tasks[ 54, 55 ]. In conclusion, results point toward different structural cerebral changes with advancing age. Grey matter volume reaches its maximum in early adulthood and then shows a relatively linear decline until region-specific accelerations or decelerations of cortical atrophy can be observed at older ages Fig.

Hypothetical model of global structural cerebral changes across the lifespan particularly based upon the results of Good et al. The diagram illustrates the different patterns of changes in grey matter volume, white matter volume, and white matter integrity FA, fractional anisotropy; MD, mean diffusivity. Relations between the different measures are not considered. Whereas global grey matter volume alterations describe a relatively linear decline until an age of 70 analog courses for cortical thinning and brain tissue surface , white matter volume and integrity show inverted U-shaped patterns with peaks in different decades of life.

The blue dashed line indicates that there may be accelerated grey matter atrophy at older ages with region-specific differences [ 24, 25 ]. By contrast white matter volume and integrity show inverted U-shaped patterns with peaks after the third and the fifth decade of life and accelerated changes in early and late life decades. The ratio between grey and white matter atrophy appears to be relatively balanced. In fact, there are studies reporting more pronounced white matter than grey matter changes in older individuals while other findings suggest the opposite pattern [ 44 ].

Interestingly, the different peaks in the course of white matter changes indicate that white matter volume and white matter integrity may be complementary measures for the status of white matter [ 41, 56 ], since white matter integrity already declines when white matter volume reaches its maximum.

Article Tags

The described patterns of structural decline can manifest in various brain regions, although prefrontal structures show a specific vulnerability to age-related degeneration. This frontal vulnerability applies to both grey and white matter degeneration and leads to cognitive decline in different cognitive domains such as executive functioning, attention, memory, and working memory. Other important reasons for age-related changes in cognition are functional cerebral changes with advancing age.

Contrary to structural imaging approaches, neurophysiological methods as electroencephalography EEG or magnetoencephalography MEG as well as functional imaging techniques as functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI provide the opportunity to examine neural activation during the performance of a specific cognitive task.

Mayo Clinic Minute: How Alzheimer’s disease affects personality

Thereby, EEG and MEG for reviews, see [ 57, 58 ] provide a high temporal resolution that enables direct recording of neural activity, whereas fMRI has a high spatial resolution allowing a quite accurate localization of activation albeit slightly delayed. Hence, specific cognitive sub-processes can directly be attributed to the activation of specific brain regions.

Noteworthy, there is a great overlap between the neural networks associated with cognitive domains as memory, attention, or executive functioning. Episodic and semantic memory processes e. All of these cognitive domains are associated with prefrontal cortex activation and the same applies to working memory operations. Working memory probably is one of the best explored functions in healthy aging and many of the findings on its mechanisms seem to be applicable to other cognitive domains as well. Working memory is associated with neural activation of an anterior-posterior network particularly including prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices [ 36, 62 ].

Advancing age is associated with activation changes within this network, whereby prefrontal brain regions, equivalent to age-related changes in brain structure, seem to show a specific functional vulnerability. In fact, numerous studies reported prefrontal activation differences between younger and older individuals [ 63, 64 ]. The kind of these differences, however, varies across studies and can manifest in quantitative or qualitative differences.

In addition to the heterogeneity of these differences, their interpretation is subject to a high degree of variability. Frontal hypoactivation, for example, was interpreted as high neural efficiency or a reduction of neural resources neural dysfunction , whereas frontal hyperactivation or increased bilaterality were attributed to neural inefficiency, neural compensation or reduced regional specificity [ 63, 65—79 ].

Reasons for these apparently inconsistent interpretations may be found in differing study designs different paradigms, methods, etc.


  • Primary Nav.
  • Cool Season Gardener: Extend the Harvest, Plan Ahead, and Grow Vegetables Year-Round!
  • Introduction.
  • ALIEN BEACH?
  • Shared Leadership: Reframing the Hows and Whys of Leadership.
  • Signs and symptoms of Alzheimer's disease?

Moreover, the kind of activation differences between older and younger individuals is not only dependent from age but also from numerous other factors. Performance accuracy and task load. Obviously, age-related activation differences cannot be interpreted without considering the impact of performance accuracy which is why brain data results were controlled for accuracy effects in some studies e.

In fact, prefrontal hyperactivation or increased bilaterality associated with increased or equivalent performance accuracy clearly point toward a compensatory recruitment of neural resources. In this case, an older individual enables more neural resources to achieve the same performance as a younger individual. By contrast, prefrontal hyperactivation or increased bilaterality associated with lower performance accuracy are rather signs of neural inefficiency or reduced regional specificity. Moreover, hypoactivation together with low performance accuracy points toward limited neural resources, whereas hypoactivation in the presence of high performance accuracy suggests a high degree of neural efficiency.

Another factor modulating age-related activation differences is the load level of the applied task. In fact, pronounced age effects can be identified by paradigms in which task demands are experimentally manipulated. On the behavioral level, an increase of task demands requires the recruitment of additional resources which are often limited in older individuals. On the neural level, these restraints are reflected by activation differences between older and younger individuals particularly within prefrontal cortex, as revealed by both imaging and neurophysiological data [ 79—84 ].

The CRUNCH model postulates that older individuals show unimpaired working memory performances at low task demands but frontal hyperactivation or increased bilaterality reflecting a recruitment of additional neural resources to compensate for an age-related reduction of working memory capacity. At high task demands, by contrast, older individuals show poorer performances than younger individuals as well as frontal hypoactivation indicating a limitation of neural resources. The CRUNCH assumptions could be replicated in numerous studies [ 69, 80—83 ], so that there is growing evidence for a double dissociation of load-related activation between older and younger adults.

Toepper and colleagues, for example, used fMRI to examine spatial working memory related brain activation in 45 healthy volunteers between 20 and 68 years of age [ 85 ]. Working memory load was manipulated by the number of displayed target stimuli. Older subjects showed higher prefrontal activation at low task load than at high task load, whereas younger participants showed the opposite pattern.

Moreover, older participants showed more activation than younger participants at low task load and less activation than younger participants at high task load Fig. The diagrams illustrate activation intensity at low, medium, and high task load dependent from age within the peak of this cluster and two study-independent peaks identified by conjunction analyses across different age groups and load levels [ 69 ] and across verbal and nonverbal working memory tasks [ 62 ]. Results showed analogue activation patterns for all three peaks indicating increasing activation intensity with advancing age at low load and decreasing activation intensity with advancing age at high load as well as higher activation intensity in older compared to younger individuals at low load and lower activation intensity in older compared to younger individuals at high load double dissociation.

The results confirm the CRUNCH assumptions indicating that the recruitment of neural resources is impaired with advancing age. Moreover, the results argue for a linearly increasing neural dysfunction with an onset in early adulthood. Contrary to the CRUNCH assumptions, however, the findings indicate that prefrontal hyperactivation at low task load may also reflect neural inefficiency rather than compensation.

High- versus low-performers. Another option to examine the impact of behavioral performance on brain activation is the comparison between high- and low-performing individuals in different age cohorts. An fMRI study of Bauer and colleagues, for example, revealed that older high-performers similar as younger high-performers showed increasing prefrontal activation with increasing working memory load [ 86 ]. Younger low-performers also showed increased activation at medium task load, but no further increase at high task load.

Consequently, neural resources of younger low-performers appeared to be exhausted earlier compared to younger and older high-performing subjects. The poorest performances were shown by older low-performers. In addition, older low-performers did not show an increased neural response to increasing task load suggesting that older low-performing subjects already reached their resource ceiling at low task demands Fig.

Hypothetical model of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex DLPFC activation modulated by age and performance level particularly based upon the results of Nagel et al. OHP show a similar though less differentiated pattern indicating a flexible recruitment of neural resources similar to that of YHP.

Please notice that qualitative group differences are shown; exact quantitative relations were not considered. Very similar effects of performance level on the recruitment of neural resources were previously reported by Nagel and colleagues [ 69 ], although a different paradigm, different methods, and different load levels were utilized e.

Contrary to the results of Bauer and colleagues, however, Nagel and colleagues reported a quadratic trend in older low-performers within right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex suggesting that older low-performers show increased activation at low load levels before activation intensity reaches a plateau and decreases at high task load. Despite this difference, the two studies provide a good example for the validity and replicability of complex aging effects [ 87 ], particularly in the context of the ongoing discussion about the limited reproducibility of psychological studies [ 88 ].

Besides age-related changes in neural activation, functional cerebral changes are characterized by changes in functional connectivity. Functional connectivity describes the degree of how much activation in one brain region is correlated with activation in another brain region and therefore provides a measure of intracerebral communication. Particularly, frontal functional connections are associated with performance accuracy [ 70, 89 ] and appear to be defective in older adults. This frontal connectivity dysfunction implies impaired bifrontal, frontoparietal, and frontostriatal connections [ 90—92 ].

Toepper and colleagues, for example, used fMRI during a spatial working memory task and found a more diffuse connectivity pattern in older compared to younger adults [ 93 ] indicating less specific representations and neural circuits [ 94, 95 ]. Moreover, older adults showed reduced functional connectivity between dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices associated with a disproportional accuracy decline on the behavioral level.

These findings suggest that the functional integrity of frontal neural circuits is reduced in older adults leading to less flexible and efficient working memory control processes [ 95—97 ]. Hubs facilitate the communication within and between different network modules and enable a flexible and efficient intracerebral communication. In healthy seniors, one of these hubs is located in orbitofrontal cortex [ 98 ].

Age-related orbitofrontal atrophy [ 4, 30 ] may damage those hubs and, as a consequence, lead to frontal connectivity dysfunctions and impaired cognitive performance. Noteworthy, frontal connectivity changes may not only manifest in decreased but also in increased functional connectivity. For example, some studies point toward increased frontal connectivity as sign of neural compensation [ 99 ] or dedifferentiation [ 57 ]. Such results suggest that factors as performance accuracy or task load do not only modulate age-related changes in neural activation but also in functional connectivity.

In fact, higher functional connectivity between different prefrontal brain regions and between prefrontal and parietal brain regions does not only seem to be associated with higher performance accuracy but also with increasing task demands [ 70, 89, , ]. Younger individuals and high-performing seniors, for example, showed increased functional connectivity as neural response to increased task load, whereas low-performing seniors did not [ 70 ].

Consequently, age-related changes in functional connectivity seem to follow a similar pattern as changes in activation [ 70, ]. Moreover, the functional connectivity results argue for a specific vulnerability of the prefrontal cortex and particularly highlight the relevance of rostral and dorsolateral prefrontal regions for age-related cognitive decline.

Noteworthy, age-related functional cerebral changes also include changes in neurotransmission. Among these changes, the reduction of dopamine seems to be the most relevant factor for reduced cognitive performance [ — ]. In particular, there is strong evidence for an age-related dysfunction of the nigrostriatal dopamine system as reflected by the reduction of pre- and postsynaptic receptor densities i. With advancing age, impaired dopaminergic modulation leads to impaired cognitive performance in multiple domains, particularly including processing speed, episodic memory, cognitive control, and working memory [ ].

Thereby, most studies reported a linear decrease of dopamine with advancing age, although there is also evidence for an accelerated decline in later decades of life [ ]. In general, the relation between dopamine and cognition describes an inverted U-shaped pattern with low and high levels of dopamine being associated with poor cognitive performance [ ]. In fact, the best dopamine level seems to be reached in early adulthood, whereas too low levels in old age or too high levels in adolescence may result in cognitive dysfunctions. One possible reason for reduced dopamine in older adults certainly is the loss of dopamine cell bodies in the substantia nigra across the lifespan [ ].

Future research must show how the correlative triad between dopamine, cognition, and age is related to other factors such as structural integrity or functional connectivity, for example. Age-related changes in brain function include activation changes in rostral parts of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, reduced functional connections to other frontal, parietal, and striatal brain regions as well as impaired dopaminergic transmission.

Importantly, changes in activation and functional connectivity are modulated by different factors such as performance accuracy or task load. However, future research studies should consider the impact of other factors as well. One of these factors is the specific localization of age-related activation changes. In fact, the prefrontal cortex can be divided into different hierarchically organized functional modules that may be differentially affected by advancing age.

The most prominent theories on this topic either propose a dorsolateral-ventrolateral [ — ] or a rostral-caudal [ , ] prefrontal cortex organization with dorsolateral or rostral parts being associated with higher-level executive processes, whereas ventrolateral or caudal parts are rather related to the relatively passive maintenance of information [ , ]. Thereby, increasing age seems to affect activation particularly within dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or rostral prefrontal cortex, respectively[ 85, 86, , ].

In conclusion, healthy aging goes along with structural and functional cerebral changes. Structural changes comprise both grey and white matter atrophy, while functional changes include changes in activation, functional connectivity, and neurotransmission. All of these changes are associated with decreased cognitive performance in older adults.

Moreover, numerous studies point toward a specific vulnerability of the prefrontal cortex with advancing age.

What is Alzheimer's Disease? | CDC

Future research should focus on the specific relations between structural and functional alterations. Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, for example, argued that regional hyperactivation in older individuals may only indicate compensatory processes, if this hyperactivation was associated with increased cognitive performance and likewise with structural decline [ 79 ]. Otherwise, it would remain unclear what older individuals must actually compensate for. In this context, some studies showed that the structural integrity of frontal fiber connections was associated with activation intensity in prefrontal brain regions as well as with cognitive performance accuracy [ 10, ].

In fact, only the methodical combination of MRI-, DTI-, and behavioral measures may allow valid conclusions on the specific mechanisms underlying age-related cognitive deterioration [ 43, 78 ]. Age-related changes in brain structure and function go along with impaired cognitive performance. Particularly, reduced processing speed may impede certain activities in everyday life.

However, these cognitive changes do not severely affect basic routines e. By contrast, neurodegenerative disorders are characterized by severe cognitive deficits eventually leading to a complete loss of autonomy. AD is characterized by pathological cerebral changes that already manifest in the fifth decade of life long before first cognitive changes become apparent or structural atrophy can be objectified by MRI techniques [ , ].

Consequently, AD takes a long subclinical course and may therefore not necessarily be regarded as a typical geriatric disease. Nevertheless, age is the main risk factor for AD which leads to exponentially increasing incidence and prevalence rates across the lifespan. The course of AD can be subdivided into different stages with a total duration between five to eight years[ , ]. Across the different disease stages, the formation of plaques and tangles follow specific patterns [ , ]: Plaques first manifest in the temporal lobe and the orbitofrontal cortex to expand to different cortical association cortices as well as to hippocampus, amygdala, diencephalon, and basal ganglia.

In later disease stages, mesencephalon, brain stem, and cerebellum are affected. Neurofibrillary tangles manifest in the locus coeruleus as well as in entorhinal and transentorhinal cortices. Afterwards, hippocampus and frontal brain regions are affected, before neurofibrillary changes involve all cortical areas in later disease stages. The formation of plaques and tangles is caused by different biochemical processes. Eventually, this cascade leads to cerebral tissue damage, synaptic failure, and cell death [ — ]. Noteworthy, AD can be rather regarded as a synaptic dysfunction than as a neural dysfunction since the loss of synapses seems to exceed the loss of neurons and represents the strongest correlate of dementia[ , ].

Noteworthy, the extent of the described pathologies plaques and tangles can be quantified by an analysis of cerebrospinal fluid CSF. Neurofibrillary tangles, by contrast, mainly consist of hyperphosphorylated tau p-tau protein, additionally leading to an increased p-tau concentration [ , ]. Finally, as a consequence of the neuronal injury caused by these events, the total-tau t-tau protein concentration in the CSF of AD patients is increased as well [ , , , ]. As mentioned above, the main consequence of these AD-related pathological changes are cognitive deficits. During the progression of the disease, these deficits lead to restricted daily routines [ ].

Both cognitive deficits and restricted daily routines belong to the main criteria of AD diagnosis. The first AD-related cognitive symptoms are episodic memory deficits [ , ] particularly involving the learning of new information. As a consequence, AD patients typically show poor retrieval and recognition performances at learning tasks [ 5, — ]. The main reason for these deficits is an encoding dysfunction that impedes the consolidation of new information into long-term memory and with that the formation of cerebral representations.

As a consequence, AD patients are not able to recall learning content. Moreover, they do not profit from cues typically indicated by a high number of false-positive responses during item recognition. Impaired retrieval and recognition performances are the first signs of an AD-related episodic memory dysfunction and can therefore be regarded as the cardinal symptoms of AD-related cognitive decline. Besides episodic memory, semantic memory is affected in early AD stages often being reflected by word-finding difficulties [ , ]. Due to the degeneration of semantic networks, AD patients have restricted access to semantic memory content [ — ].

As a consequence, they typically show poor performances in naming tasks that require the identification of objects. Toepper and colleagues [ ], for example, qualitatively analyzed the answers of 40 AD patients and 40 healthy controls in the Boston Naming Test [ ]. Besides pronounced naming deficits in AD patients, results showed that the semantic proximity of the answers to a displayed object was associated with semantic network integrity and global cognitive functioning.

Moreover, there was evidence for a top-down deterioration of semantic network integrity in AD, beginning with higher-order categories. Other cognitive domains being affected by AD are orientation [ 19, ] and executive functioning [ — ], the latter of which may involve specific sub-processes as abstract thinking, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition [ — ]. Regarding inhibitory deficits, particularly the active or intentional inhibition of irrelevant information appears to be impaired in AD [ , ].

Further progression of the disease then leads to impaired attention, albeit attentional processes as processing speed appear to be longer preserved from AD-related change than other cognitive domains [ , ]. Importantly, all of the cognitive deficits described above also affect higher-level operations.

Common examples are symbol comprehension [ ] or driving skills [ ], both of which contribute to autonomy in everyday life. Late AD stages are then associated with aphasia, apraxia, behavioral disorders, affective symptoms, and motor dysfunctions. Due to immunodeficiency and dysphagia, AD patients are at some point particularly vulnerable to respiratory infections representing the most frequent cause of death.

The progression of AD-related cognitive deterioration appears to be slower in early and late disease stages than in middle stages [ ], although there is also evidence for linear decline [ ]. Disease progression is characterized by high between-subject variability. Number and duration of those plateaus are also subject to high variance. The typical deficit profile in AD, however, is characterized by pronounced memory deficits episodic and semantic representing the cognitive key symptom of the disease. In addition, early executive and orientation deficits can be observed, whereas processing speed is relatively preserved until further progression of the disease.

Reasons for these deficits are structural and functional cerebral changes including grey and white matter degeneration as well as changes in neural activation, functional connectivity and neurotransmission. The volume loss follows a similar pattern as the expansion of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles as described above [ ].

Volume loss initially affects hippocampus and entorhinal cortex [ 43, ]. In fact, there is metaanalytical evidence that patients in the prodromal stage of AD exclusively show grey matter volume reductions in these brain regions [ , ]. Hippocampal volume loss is associated with episodic memory deficits representing the cognitive cardinal symptom of AD [ ].

For AD-related entorhinal atrophy, even higher annual reduction rates arereported. Following these mediotemporal volume reductions, grey matter atrophy affects different association cortices [ 43, , ]. Thereby, structural volume reductions spread from middle and inferior temporal gyri, inferior parietal lobe, and posterior cingulate cortex to other parts of the parietal lobe, frontal brain regions, and finally to anterior parts of the cingulate cortex [ ]. Noteworthy, posterior parietal and posterior cingulate atrophy seem to be associated with an earlier onset and a faster progression of the disease [ ].

By contrast, occipital brain regions as well as primary sensory and motor regions seem to be relatively preserved from AD-related grey matter atrophy. Contrary to healthy seniors who show a specific vulnerability of the prefrontal cortex and a relatively preserved integrity of the hippocampal formation, AD patients show the opposite pattern [ , ].

In fact, Head and colleagues suggested a fronto-hippocampal dissociation between healthy aging and AD, which was supported by the results of Jack and colleagues who also reported rather discrete frontal atrophy in AD patients, whereas pronounced volume reductions were found in mediotemporal brain regions and the parietotemporal junction. Although AD is mainly characterized by grey matter atrophy, white matter is also affected. Similar to grey matter volume reductions, white matter volume loss particularly involves mediotemporal structures as hippocampus and entorhinal cortex [ ].

Moreover, white matter atrophy in inferior parietal, temporal, and anterior frontal brain regions as well as in posterior cingulate cortex, posterior corpus callosum, and cerebellum are reported [ , ]. Some of these changes already manifest in prodromal stages of the disease [ ]. Moreover, AD-related white matter changes are characterized by reduced structural integrity decreased FA and increased MD. These changes particularly include intracortical projections in temporal, parietal and frontal brain regions, whereas extracortical fiber systems as sensory or motor projections are relatively preserved [ 43, , ].

Again, these changes particularly affect hippocampal, parahippocampal, and entorhinal connections [ — ] but also posterior cingulate cortex, posterior corpus callosum [ , ], anterior temporal lobe [ ], and fasciculus longitudinalis superior [ ]. Consistent with the results on AD-related grey matter changes, DTI results do not suggest a specific vulnerability of frontal brain regions. Instead, there is evidence for a posterior-anterior gradient of structural degeneration with a specific vulnerability of posterior structures and a relative preservation of the frontal brain [ , ].

This gradient was observed for reduced FA as well as for increased MD. Moreover, there is evidence for an association between white matter integrity and cognition [ ]. In fact, global and frontal white matter integrity as well as integrity of fornix, cingulum, cingulate cortex, and corpus callosum were shown to be correlated with global cognitive performance.

In addition, there is evidence for a relationship between regional white matter integrity and specific cognitive subfunctions. Episodic memory deficits, for example, are particularly associated with structural integrity of cingulum, fornix, and fasciculus uncinatus. Moreover, associations between memory retrieval deficits and reduced structural integrity in temporal and posterior cingulate cortices, between executive dysfunctions and reduced frontal integrity and between impaired nonverbal memory or verbal fluency and integrity of the corpus callosum were reported [ , , ].

Altogether, the degeneration of fiber connections between mediotemporal structures and corpus callosum or frontal brain regions seem to be of particular relevance for AD-related cognitive decline [ , ]. Thereby, contrary to healthy seniors, more robust correlations between MD and cognitive performance than between FA and cognitive performance were observed in AD patients, indicating that the increase of MD seems to be more important for AD-related cognitive decline than the decrease in FA[ 43, , ].

In conclusion, the results on AD-related changes in brain structure point toward a posterior-anterior gradient of grey and white matter degeneration including a specific vulnerability of the medial temporal lobe and posterior parts of the corpus callosum. Fiber tracts connecting mediotemporal brain structures with the corpus callosum and the frontal lobe appear to be particularly vulnerable and their degeneration leads to increasing cognitive deficits during the progression of the disease.

Future research should specify the association between grey and white matter atrophy. Grey matter volume loss and reductions of white matter integrity, for example, seem to show similar patterns [ ]. Moreover, Villain and colleagues [ ] showed that hippocampal grey matter volume loss was associated with reduced white matter integrity of the cingulum which again was correlated with hypometabolism in the right temporoparietal association cortex and different structures of the Papez circuit posterior cingulate cortex, thalamus, mamillary bodies, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus.

Moreover, there is evidence for an association between hippocampal grey matter volume and FA values of the fasciculus uncinatus which again are associated with episodic memory deficits [ ]. These results suggest that an AD-related hippocampal disconnection is linked to hippocampal atrophy and contributes to early episodic memory deficits. In fact, only the methodological combination of different measures may allow more specific conclusions on the mechanisms leading to cognitive decline in AD. Next to structural cerebral changes, AD is characterized by functional changes. Neurophysiological studies consistently revealed altered neural activity in patients with AD or MCI, being associated with dysfunctions in various cognitive domains such as memory, attention, working memory or spatial abilities [ 57, — ].

Since memory processes are initially affected by AD, first activation changes can be observed during memory tasks. Sperling, for example, found reduced mediotemporal activation i.

By contrast, increased mediotemporal activation was reported for memory retrieval possibly reflecting successful compensation [ ], which is consistent with the assumption that mediotemporal hyperactivation can particularly be observed in early AD stages. Modulated by performance accuracy, however, this hyperactivation may reflect both successful [ ] or inefficient compensatory mechanisms [ ]. The latter finding goes better with studies reporting an association between early mediotemporal hyperactivation and faster cognitive decline [ ].

Further progression of the disease then results in increasingly reduced mediotemporal activation and aggravating memory deficits [ ]. Noteworthy, memory deficits seem to be associated with activation changes in other cortical areas as well. In fact, there is evidence for AD-related dysfunctional hypoactivation in prefrontal and parietal cortices as well as for compensatory hyperactivation in prefrontal cortex [ 43, , ].

Besides memory deficits, there is evidence for early working memory and executive dysfunctions associated with activation changes particularly in prefrontal brain regions. Similar to the results on memory processes, compensatory prefrontal hyperactivation [ — ] as well as dysfunctional prefrontal hypoactivation [ , ] were reported. As mentioned earlier, the kind of activation changes is modulated by performance accuracy and task load.

Rosano and colleagues, for example, showed that prodromal AD patients showed an upregulation of parietal brain regions as neural response to increasing task load, whereas healthy seniors showed a frontal upregulation [ ]. However, the specific effects of performance accuracy and task load are less explored in AD than in healthy aging, because fMRI experiments often turn out to overextend AD patients already in prodromal stages of the disease. Consequently, fMRI studies with AD patients often utilize resting-state paradigms most of which revealed AD-related activation changes particularly in the hippocampus and the posterior cingulate cortex [ 43, ].

With the advancement of neuroimaging techniques, AD is more and more regarded as a disconnection syndrome. In fact, there is growing evidence for impaired functional connectivity of neural networks in AD patients [ — ].

Upcoming Program:

As in healthy seniors, a possible reason for this disconnection is the degeneration of hubs [ — ]. These hubs appear to be particularly vulnerable to AD pathology [ ], which may lead to reduced functional connectivity within task-specific networks and to impaired cognitive performance [ 98 ]. For example, AD-related parietal brain atrophy i. However, frontal and hippocampal connections are also affected.

Agosta and colleagues reported functional connectivity changes within frontal neural circuits associated with executive dysfunctions [ ]. Other findings revealed reduced hippocampal connectivity to posterior parietal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and other brain regions being particularly associated with episodic memory deficits [ , ].

Finally, functional connectivity changes can be observed in the default network as well [ , , ]. Altogether, the functional connectivity results suggest a specific vulnerability of parietal and hippocampal brain regions, whereas frontal connections appear to be less affected. Dependent on moderating variables as performance accuracy or task-load, AD-related connectivity changes may manifest in either increased or decreased connectivity reflecting either dysfunction or compensation [ , , ].

Importantly, AD-related functional cerebral changes also include changes in neurotransmission. Among these changes, a cholinergic dysfunction represents the most relevant factor for AD-related cognitive decline. It is assumed that the degeneration of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain impedes synaptic processes in the hippocampus and other cortical brain regions and thus leads to memory loss including deficient encoding, consolidation, and retrieval processes [ , — ].

Cholinergic abnormalities can be manifold including impaired acetylcholine transport or release, for example. Within the basal forebrain, the degeneration of neurons in the Nucleus basalis Meynert [ , ] is of particular relevance, because this brain structure represents one of the most important nuclei in the central nervous system for the production of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine. To counteract this progressive cholinergic deficit, cholinesterase inhibitors are utilized. They enhance cholinergic neurotransmission by increasing the availability of acetylcholine in the synaptic cleft, may decelerate or even stabilize cognitive decline for one or two years but do not ensure a definite cure [ — ].

In conclusion, AD is characterized by changes in neural activation, functional connectivity, and neurotransmission. Changes in activation and functional connectivity particularly affect mediotemporal but also parietal and frontal brain regions and may manifest in both increased and decreased activation and connectivity.

Future research should focus on specific variables moderating the kind and direction of AD-related changes, such as performance accuracy or task load. Moreover, the methodological combination of activation, functional connectivity, and neurotransmission measures should be of particular interest. Taken together, AD pathology leads to grey and white matter degeneration as well as to changes in neural activation, functional connectivity, and neurotransmission.

All of these changes appear to interact leading to cognitive decline. Although many of these changes also manifest in prefrontal brain regions, imaging results rather point toward a posterior-anterior gradient of functional and structural deterioration with a specific vulnerability of the medial temporal lobe.

Future research endeavors should focus on the association between functional and structural measures. Zhou and colleagues, for example, showed that reduced connectivity between hippocampus and posterior cingulate cortex is reflected by both functional and structural imaging data [ ]. Other studies reported an association between hippocampal grey matter volume loss and reduced hippocampal activation [ , ] or connectivity [ ] or between specific CSF markers and white matter integrity [ , ].

Putcha and colleagues reported a strong inverted relationship between hippocampal activation and volume in the prodromal stage of the disease MCI [ ], while Huijbers and colleagues presented longitudinal results indicating that hippocampal volume and activation independently predicted clinical progression in patients with MCI [ ]. The two latter studies suggest that activation changes are certainly associated with, but not entirely determined by structural deterioration.

However, only the methodological combination of all of the described pathological markers and their relation to cognitive performance may allow more specific conclusions on the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying AD pathology. But this can vary considerably from person to person. Alzheimer's disease is a life-limiting illness, although many people diagnosed with the condition will die from another cause.

As Alzheimer's disease is a progressive neurological condition, it can cause problems with swallowing. This can lead to aspiration food being inhaled into the lungs , which can cause frequent chest infections. It's also common for people with Alzheimer's disease to eventually have difficulty eating and have a reduced appetite. As the exact cause of Alzheimer's disease is not clear, there's no known way to prevent the condition. But there are things you can do that may reduce your risk or delay the onset of dementia, such as:. These measures have other health benefits, such as lowering your risk of cardiovascular disease and improving your overall mental health.

Read more about preventing Alzheimer's disease. There are dozens of dementia research projects going on around the world, many of which are based in the UK. If you have a diagnosis of dementia or are worried about memory problems, you can help scientists better understand the disease by taking part in research. If you have been diagnosed with dementia, or you're caring for someone with the condition, remember that advice and support is available to help you live well.

Staying independent with dementia. Communicating with people with dementia. Dementia, social services and the NHS. Our guide to care and support explains your options and where you can get support. Page last reviewed: 10 May Next review due: 10 May Alzheimer's disease is the most common type of dementia in the UK. These include: increasing age a family history of the condition untreated depression , although depression can also be one of the symptoms of Alzheimer's disease lifestyle factors and conditions associated with cardiovascular disease Read more about the causes of Alzheimer's disease.

Signs and symptoms of Alzheimer's disease Alzheimer's disease is a progressive condition, which means the symptoms develop gradually over many years and eventually become more severe. The first sign of Alzheimer's disease is usually minor memory problems. Who is affected? Alzheimer's disease is most common in people over the age of If Alzheimer's disease is suspected, you may be referred to a specialist service to: assess your symptoms in more detail organise further testing, such as brain scans if necessary create a treatment and care plan Read more about diagnosing Alzheimer's disease.

How Alzheimer's disease is treated There's currently no cure for Alzheimer's disease, but medicines are available that can help relieve some of the symptoms. Outlook People with Alzheimer's disease can live for several years after they start to develop symptoms. This includes support for families, as well as the person with Alzheimer's.

Can Alzheimer's disease be prevented? Dementia research There are dozens of dementia research projects going on around the world, many of which are based in the UK. If you're a carer for someone with dementia, you can also take part in research. More information Dementia can affect all aspects of a person's life, as well as their family's.

Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View
Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View
Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View
Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View
Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View
Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View

Related Alzheimer Disease: The Changing View: The Changing View



Copyright 2019 - All Right Reserved